Context sensibility and modality

distinguish environmental design from empirical research

 

Prof.dr.ir. Taeke M. de Jong, Faculty of Architecture, University of technology Delft

Berlageweg 1 Delft 2628 CR The Netherlands.

Email T.M.deJong@bk.tudelft.nl

 

Contents

 

Abstract 1

1   Introduction. 2

2   Possible, probable and desirable futures. 3

3   Different language games. 4

4   Context sensibility, a matter of scale. 5

5   Types of design research and research by design. 7

6   Making a drawing a scientific document 8

7   Conclusion. 9

References. 9

Key words. 10

 

 

Abstract

In a university of technology, designs are made (research by design) or examined and evaluated (design research). Making a design, the preliminary investigation and its conclusion, the programme of demands, only partly direct the solution. The design does not follow unequivocally and reproductably from a programme like a scientific prediction of some probable future from its basic assumptions ceteris paribus. Even with a strict programme, many alternatives (eventually unexpectable) are possible in design. This is most explicit in urban and building design. The choice of a final alternative is more determined by the context of the object to be designed. The market, the location and the designer (context of invention) belong to the broader present and future managerial, cultural, economical, technical, ecological, and mass-space-time context and its probable change (perspective) of the object. ‘Context’ is different on different levels of scale and cannot be foreseen completely in the programme.

 

Even in a determined context and perspective the number of imaginable alternatives for buildings, mostly with a long term multifunctional programme of (conflicting) demands, is unconceivably large, subject to a combinatoric explosion of possible futures.

Probable futures are always possible, but not all possible futures are also probable. The difference between technical and other sciences is a matter of modality. Predicting (empirical) research produces probable futures by counting chance from experience, design produces possible futures, especially when they are not yet probable, by drawing possible events. Experimental empirical research can also produce unexpected possible futures, however only because it presupposes the design of the experiment and its instruments. Any designed or realised artifact can be studied empirically (impact analysis before or after realization, ex ante or ex post), but how do we study it, before it is designed? A programme for (environmental) design can be composed empirically, but it only gives direction in a limited amount of design-decisions. Otherwise a design would not be a design, but a prediction. Predicting and designing require different language games with confusion of tongues between the games. This article represents key concepts from (Jong, Cuperus et al. 2000) the temporary conference version of a book with 40 authors to be published this year and from (Jong 1992). The article ends with some suggestions to make these concepts operational for design research and research by design. It discusses a database making designs retrievable as scientific documents concerning their context, perspective and possible impacts.

 

Keywords: design (architectural, urban and technical), research, context sensibility, modality, scale.

 


1.a      Introduction

Architectural, urban and technical design of buildings and their settings are more context-sensible than other technical arts. That raises a methodological problem of scientific generalization, well known also in ecology as a context-science pre-eminently (Pianka 1994) and in management sciences(Riemsdijk 1999). How to scientify something so elusive as ‘context’ as set of conditions of possibility?

This article is written in the language game of the ‘possible’, containing the narrower ‘probable’. It stresses conditions instead of causes. A cause is always a condition something to happen, but not every condition is also a cause. So the foundation of a building could be a condition of the building, but not its cause. The combinatorial explosion of possible forms even within one strict urban or architectural empirically produced program, raises a problem in that sense. It is also the problem of naming and describing the innumerable possible results of design and consequently the problem of their retrievability. A Windows-icon of 16x16 pixels with 256 colors makes possible much more pictures (256256) than there are atoms in the universe (less than 10125). How many possibilities than has a plot of 100m2 to be filled with 1000 alternative building materials? The designer travels through that multiple universe, only able to name some of the particles (s)he passes. After all, the number of words available in any language to cover this multitude never exceeds 108. Only drawn icons as legend to a larger drawing can approach it. How could we scientify the drawing and its legend, its vocabulary? Here the concept of scale, limited by grain (smallest drawn particle) and frame of the drawing plays an important role.

 

Buildings and urban designs have a long period of use and they are earthbound. So they have to function in a changing context (perspective) that is unpredictable and not influenced by the programming authority, designer or user. From the viewpoint of their durability they should be able to accommodate varying programmes and daily changing aims of their inhabitants and users. This quality of building design is called ‘robustness’. ‘Flexibility’ is only part of it, multifunctionality another part. So, from all artefacts, buildings have the most context sensitive function for use, perception and market, not to be evaluated without that context and therefore hardly comparable to each other (sometimes even unique). In this field it is difficult to find comparable examples for research to draw more general conclusions for design.

Even with a comparable programme of demands, not only their own diversity of solutions, but also the diversity of their contexts or perspectives to function in, is very large. Consequently, the diversity of rational reasons (determined by context) to choose a final alternative is even larger. So, building design research often has the character of an n=1 study with limited general value to other designs. Design research, based on more examples than one, is often ignored by designers, because on location many design relevant circumstances appear different from what the examined examples had in common. The descriptive interpretation of context by researchers differs from the imaginative interpretation of designers stressing possibilities rather than probabilities. Moreover, the principal often asks for a unique design, ‘exploiting’ rare qualities of context. So design decisions seldom can be founded on examples univocally and professionally by the lack of material for comparison.

 

Predicting (empirical) research produces probable futures by counting chance from experience. Design produces possible futures, especially when they are not yet probable, by drawing possible events. Experimental empirical research can also produce unexpected possible futures, however only because it presupposes the design of the experiment[a] and its instruments[b].

This article firstly stresses this difference, alerting different language games. Then it makes the concept of context and perspective operational for research by scale articulation. It distinguishes four types of design research and research by design on the basis of object and context being determined or variable. At last it asks attention for an important condition for these types of research: the retievability of the results of design on the basis of specified context, perspective and readable impacts.

1.b      Possible, probable and desirable futures

Possibility as such, contains more than what is actually possible, it contains anything that ever could be invented. However it contains less then what is imaginable, for instance science fiction. The boundaries of the imaginable we cannot draw, because the other side of the imaginable is principally not imaginable (Wittgenstein 1922). The boundary of the possible we can imagine, though we do not know it. This oppresses in design research, biology, ecology and management science. Kauffman (Kauffman 2000) states: ‘In statistical mechanics, with its famous liter box of gas as an isolated thermodynamic system, we can prestate the configuration space of all possible positions and momenta of the gas particles in the box. …  What if we cannot prestate the configuration space of a biosphere …’. In that case Laplace statistics is not a useful instrument. Probability partly contains logical necessity and truth. Necessary or true is per definition probable, but probable is not always necessary or true. However it can become true. The boundary of probability is drawn by choice of the percentile, for instance the 99% percentile. The not probable 1% looks small, but in the combinatoric explosion of possibilities certainly the unexpected and ceteris non paribus, it is very large. In design it is the object of attention pre-eminently. Otherwise design would be prediction.

 

The difference between research and design is the difference in modality between probable and possible. The set of probable and therefore empirically researchable futures[c] is part of a much larger set of possible futures (Figure 1). Probable futures can be predicted because they are probable, but how do we explore the possibilities to the extent that they are improbable? These must be designed.

 

 

Figure 1 - Modalities, their corresponding activities and basis of reasoning

 

This inclusive distinction also refers to a difference in language of causal and conditional reasoning.

A cause is only a cause under well-defined conditions. When a journal reports: ‘The collision was caused by the inattention of one of the drivers’, a physicist could state: ‘Nonsens! The collision was caused by two objects approaching eachother with high speed’. If that is true, the journal is not right, because when the two cars would not have approached eachother, the inattention would not have been the cause of a collision. The approaching was a hidden condition presupposed in the first explanation of the collision under these circumstances. The inattention as a cause was the ‘last added condition’ the collision to happen.

 

A visual representation of architectural design says more that a thousand words, but what kind of  words are these? They sum up conditions producing possibilities[d] for future users. These users choose their own multidude of activities within these conditions. For example a household is not caused by the architectural design, so it is not predicable as long as you believe in its freedom of choice. An architectural design only makes different households possible.

 

The desirable future, the set of desired events (such as for example summarised on a specific level of scale in the party political programmes) overlaps both:

 

 

Figure 2 - Possible, probable and desirable futures

 

When analysed, this domain of partly enclosing and overlapping types of future (on an other level of abstraction ‘modes’) provides 5 exclusive categories (Figure 2):

 

1.       The first category is composed by desired perspectives, but impossible according to all technical insights (science fiction). They are irrelevant to scientific research and technical study in object language. On an other level of abstraction psychological research, for instance dream research, they could be relevant.

 

2.       Futures which we do want, and are probably fulfilled, are thus per definition also possible. These futures do not demand our prime interest. It is highly desirable that the sun will rise tomorrow, but also so probable, that we do not have to call on the services of a firm of consultants in order to ensure that the sun will actually rise tomorrow.

 

3.       For the possible futures to the extent that they are not probable, but desirable indeed we must undertake some action, this is the domain of our aims.

 

4.       The probable and therefore also possible futures that we do not want are brought up by problem-detecting research. They contain the domain of our problems.

 

5.       The possible futures that are improbable and are (still) not desired by anyone until exposed by the designer. The domain of desirable futures can easily change by newly designed possibilities.

 

Empirical research confines itself to the probable futures, the design also reaches out to possible futures, most pure in as far as they are not probable. Of course any design has lots of probable components on higher and lower levels of scale, but its public appreciation does not only concern its predictability. What probably will happen, will also happen without design. Prediction (for example: ‘this spanning will hold’) is an essential, but a less characteristic part of design.

 

1.c      Different language games

Different modal domains raise different language games (Wittgenstein 1953), characterised by different professional reductions of reality (Table 1).

 

Language games:

being able

knowing

wanting

Modality:

possible

probable

desirable

Activities:

design

research

policy

Reductions as to

 

 

 

Character:

 legend

variable

agenda

Space or time:

tolerance

relations

appointments

 

Table 1 – language games and their practice

 

In every day language the games are preceded by auxiliary verbs like ‘can’, ‘is’ or ‘want to’. They warn us that the sentence to come should be interpretated in the respective mode. Otherwise the listener could blame us as a liar when (s)he interpretes our intention ‘This building can be built’ or ‘I want this building to be built’ as ‘This building will be built’. The first sentences show a possibility or  desirability, the last a probability. Any expression is an abstraction and a reduction of a presupposed reality. Here, we distinguish reductions of character and space or time of the nouns in the expression.


The primary language of design to show possibilities, is that of a drawing. When the designer records the key to symbols (legend) of his drawing, for instance red for urban areas, yellow for agriculture, and blue for water, (s)he reduces the variation within the urban area, agriculture and the water. If (s)he makes his/her drawing with pre-supposed legend unities, (s)he first selects their site and form (state of dispersion) roughly and subsequently more precisely. So, during the design process (s)he reduces the tolerances of the design for the benefit of its feasibility.


The researcher reduces reality in variables
, but does not accept that a variable may assume any arbitrary value. (S)he looks for relations between the variables in order to restrict them in their freedom of movement in order to make more precise predictions.


The policy maker reduces the problems to a few items on the agenda
 and tries to reach consensus by arrangements and appointments. In practice an agenda is often confused with the legenda of the proposal.

 

1.d      Context sensibility, a matter of scale

Architecture is more context sensible than for instance mechanical engineering. That makes scientific generalization difficult (ceteris non paribus). How could we make science aware of context?

Scientify context itself, starting with its orders of size.

 

An important difference comparing architectural and urban design with other technical design, comprises an extreme context sensibility and thus non-recurrence of architectural and urban development design interventions. Context of architectural and urban objects comprises everything outside the frame of the object considered or to be designed, which could influence it (such as the location form, the layout and function prior to the design and the social desires, the programme of demands) or vice versa. The concept of context is scale-dependent and subject to scale-paradoxes.

 

 

 

Figure 3 - scale-paradox

 

Figure 4 - naming orders of scale

 

Figure 3 shows a spatial example of concept confusion, based upon a difference in the scale of consideration (scale paradox). It is shown here that identical spatial patterns allow different conclusions to be drawn when elements are involved in the consideration using a differing scope (scale level, largest frame, smallest texture grain).

For example if in the figure above one takes one circle each time and the surroundings into consideration then one must ascertain a ‘difference’, although ‘equality’ should be ascertained when one repeatedly compares groups of seven with their surroundings. Something similar applies to the consideration from inside to outside and from outside to inside. The paradoxical concept ‘homogenous mixture’ indicates precisely which dilemma this entails: it is homogenous at a specific scale level, at a lower abstraction level it is heterogeneous.

The concept ‘clustered dispersion’, well known in Dutch urban planning, is another example. For concepts like this the question must be asked immediately: ‘which scale for clustering, and which for dispersion?’. Moreover this figure shows that such confusion of tongues is possible using a factor 3 linear scale difference. Between the grains of sand and the earth lie 7 decimals; therefore in this range there are more than 14 concept confusions lurking. So, to avoid concept confusion, premises and conclusions in an argument should concern the same scale. Therefore scales here are named with nominal values with a range.

 

FRAME

NOMINAL RADIUS

Global................................................................................................................................. ............................................................................................................................................ ..........................................................................................................................................

10000

 

Continental......................................................................................................................... ..........................................................................................................................................

3000

 

Sub-continental.................................................................................................................

1000

 

National.............................................................................................................................. ..........................................................................................................................................

300

 

Sub-national....................................................................................................................... ..........................................................................................................................................

100

 

Regional............................................................................................................................. ..........................................................................................................................................

30

 

Sub-regional....................................................................................................................... ..........................................................................................................................................

10

 

Local | District | Borough

3

 

Area | Village...................................................................................................................... ..........................................................................................................................................

1

km

Neighbourhood | Hamlet

300

 

Ensemble |.......................................................................................................................... ..........................................................................................................................................

100

 

Building complex

30

 

Building.............................................................................................................................. ..........................................................................................................................................

10

 

Building_segment..............................................................................................................

3

 

Building_part...................................................................................................................... ..........................................................................................................................................

1

m

Building_component.........................................................................................................

300

 

Superelement..................................................................................................................... ..........................................................................................................................................

100

 

Element............................................................................................................................... ..........................................................................................................................................

30

 

Subelement........................................................................................................................ ..........................................................................................................................................

10

 

Trade_materials................................................................................................................

3

 

Composition_materials

1

 

Material............................................................................................................................... ..........................................................................................................................................

<1

mm

 

Table 2 – scale articulation

This gave rise to allocation of frame and grain in an array (Table 2). They differ systematically to other scale levels by a linear factor of around 3 (Figure 4) or around 10 by surface for architectural categories, (discourses, drawings, legends, concepts and objects) in the urban development (Jong and Paasman 1998) and the technology of building (Eekhout 1998). They enable the context of the category in question to be defined outside the neighbour values.

In a first draft the grain is 10% of the frame, in a plan with specifications made to scale the grain is 0.1%.

In this array we can locate the architectural, urban or technical object accordingly its largest (frame) and smallest (grain) measure concerned. The rest is 'context'. There are external contexts outside the frameand internal within the grain.

 

Management, culture, economy. technique, ecology and the design-area of space, time and mass, are scale-dependent as well (Figure 5). In all these areas aimed (intentions) and other (unintended) impacts could be sorted by order of scale.

Impacts suppose a perspective. Administration can change between initiative and mere control, culture between experimental and traditional and so on. These changes could have a different periodicity. All of them influence the way we estimate impacts of a plan.

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

CONTEXT                                       PERSPECTIVE

 

Variable per level of scale         and period of change.

For example:                                                        tentative nationally:

 

managerial/political:   initiative            < >        laissez-faire        7 years

cultural:                   traditional           < >       experimental       15 years

economics:              growth               < >            shrinkage       30 years

technical:                 combination       < >       specialization       60 years

ecological:               heterogeneous    < >      homogeneous     120 years

mass-space-time:     concentration     < >   deconcentration    240 years

 

Figure 5 - Context and its change (perspective)

 

 

 

 

Figure 6 – a tentative example of a national perspective

 

The concept of ‘perspective’ in time (Figure 6) here exists as change of ‘context’ in space, which becomes significant when the intended and unintended impacts of a design are to be interpreted, named and estimated. In which perspective does this happen, with which plan horizon and under which assumptions with regards to external developments (an initiating or controlling government, a opportunity- or tradition directed culture, a growing or stagnating economy, a technology which is successful using function combinations or on the contrary separating functions, an increasing or decreasing ecological or mass/space/time pressure).

 

1.e      Types of design research and research by design

Design research (Table 3) concerns determined objects within determined contexts. Research by design (grey in Table 3) varies either the object (identifying design) or the context (typology) or even both (designing study):

 

 

OBJECT

 

determined

variable

CONTEXT               determined

Design Research

Identifying Design

                                  variable

Typology

Designing Study

 

Table 3 – four types of design related research

 

Identifying design (right above in Table 3) is a daily practice in each and every civil engineering office that does not work in an intuitively untraceable manner. An object must be designed for a specific context (spatial, ecological, technical, economic, cultural, and administrative). New possibilities are sought for this determined context usually summarized in a programme of demands (brief, part of the context).  As soon as the design is ready, thus also the object is determined, it can be researched empirically regarding its external (contextual) impacts, but also regarding the design means applied in the design. This is in many respects empirical design research.

After a number of design researches in varying contexts have been carried out, the same context-independent types are found. A type is not yet a model (Quincy 1788-1825), (Argan 1965): one can make many models of one type. The type is context independent. This does not mean that the context is of no importance for typology. The context is variable, and this variability therefore is also object of typological research. For each (relative) context independent type, variants or models of which the appearance may be context dependent are subsequently described.

Context also has form, different on each level of scale.Therefore context itself can become a design object on a higher level of scale. If the object and the context are alternating subject of design we call it designing study.

 

These differences have a radical impact on the research method and the manner by which the research proposals and the executed studies are assessed. In this way the theory of probabilities of concepts, such as chance and average, play a smaller role than in the empirical study of the probable. The focus transfers the boundary of a 99 percentile probability scope. High context sensibility allows less generalisation and induces differential thinking.

There are of course also similarities which make possible to determine whether a product can be considered as scientific or not. The most important is the result’s ability to be criticised.

 

1.f        Making a drawing a scientific document

A special problem in design related study and research is the language of the drawing and subsequently their retrievability.

For building design research with more general design relevant (context sensitive) conclusions, we need a database with a large number of designs and composing images to find different examples in comparable contexts. This places great demands on the possibilities of verbal and non verbal selection within such a database[e]. It has consequences on the effort of documentation per image. It has methodological implications in naming and defining countless possible architectural interventions and their effects in different contexts. For this purpose the Faculty of Architecture of the Delft Technical University (DUT) spend several years in developing an Image Archive of Architectural Interventions (IAAI), able to store thousands of images made per year in that faculty, retrievable amongst others by location, context, perspective and supposed effect within that context. Its prototype functioning on the internet now contains approximately 1500 documented images from 500 recent graduate projects equiped with logical sentence functions[f] as syntactically composed keywords. They are retrievable by alternate choice of images and keywords. The database is prepared in the future to mount 3D designs in a 3D map of the Netherlands allowing fly-through in a scenario chosen in advance, to see them at first glance in their supposed mass-space-time context. Historical images then can be recorded in their own former context and perspective.

 

Recording urban designs on a national, regional, local and technical level of scale in this archive makes these designs accessible for planning research on other universities and decision making institutions. Recording technical details of buildings makes them accessible to other technical faculties like Civil Engineering and Industrial Design in DUT, Architecture in TUE and various faculties abroad. As soon as these faculties would like to give more attention to the context-sensibility of their own design examples, other than building designs, easily can be recorded according to their level of scale. It will give the database a more general design orientation, stimulating cooperation in research between the technical faculties and decision making institutions.

 

Some suggestions to make a drawing a scientifically retrievable document are:

 

·         make the drawing comparable by scale (frame, grain) and legend

·         name context and perspective

·         to reduce naming, name families of design by naming operations O and their impact (Figure 6)

 

 

Figure 6 – examples of design operations classifying designs

 

·         make the drawing retrievable for critical review and further research

 

The Image Archive of Architectural Interventions IAAI[g], based on these principles is in development. The website refreshes itself controlling its links every night. A top 10 from the best documented 100 images will be selected as home page every month. The input by students is still a bottleneck. The application is temporarily restricted to the Faculty. A further development of IAAI comprises:

 

·         a development from mere registration to a flight simulator with different sceneries in different futures, designed by the students that will be the designers of these futures;

·         differently categorizing inputs by turning the database linguistic;

·         intelligently connecting the inputs (systematizing);

·         connecting different external expert systems for decision and design support (DS).

 

1.g      Conclusion

Modality distinguishes design from empirical research. Orientation on possible, probable and desirable futures induce different language games in the practice of design. Context sensibility of the object of design raises methodological problems of generalization. They could be overcome by context or scale-articulation and distinguishing different methods for design research, studying design, typological research and designing study.

 

Ways to study design require:

·         making the drawing retrievable as a scientific document

·         by making its frame, grain, context, perspective and readable impacts explicit;

·         stimulating creativity by viewing possibilities rather then probable or desirable futures;

·         developing the desire and ability to be criticized.

 

 

References

 

Argan, G. C. (1965). Sul concetto di tipologia architettonica. (Milaan) Progetto e destino.

Eekhout, A. C. J. M., Ed. (1998). Ontwerpmethodologie. (Delft) Delft University Press.

Jong, T. M. d. (1992). Small methodology for designing study. (Delft) http://www.bk.tudelft.nl/users/dejongt/internet/008000000.htm

Jong, T. M. d. and M. Paasman (1998). Een vocabulaire voor besluitvorming over de kaart van Nederland. (Zoetermeer)  MESO.

Jong, T. M. d., Y. Cuperus, et al., Eds. (2000). Ways to study architectural, urban and technical design. (Delft) International conference of Research by design Faculteit Bouwkunde TUD.

Kant, I. (1787). Critik der reinen Vernunft. (Riga) Johann Friedrich Hartknoch.

Kauffman, S. (2000). Investigations. (Oxford) Oxford University Press.

Kuhn, T. S. (1962). The structure of Scientific Revolutions. in: The International Encyclopedia of Unified Science. O. Neurath, R. Carnap and C. Morris. (Chicago) University of Chicago.

Pianka, E. R. (1994). Evolutionary ecology. (New York) Harper Collins College Publishers.

Quincy, Q. D. (1788-1825). Architecture Encyclopedie methodique. (Paris)

Riemsdijk, M. J. v., Ed. (1999). Dilemma's in de bedrijfskundige wetenschap. (Assen) Van Gorcum.

Wittgenstein, L. (1922). Tractatus logico-philosophicus. (London, 1971) Routledge & Kegan Paul.

Wittgenstein, L. (1953). Philosophical Investigations/Philosophische Untersuchungen. (Oxford) Blackwell.


Key words

 


agenda, 5

aims, 4

appointments, 5

area, 6

borough, 6

brief, 7

building, 6

building design, 8

building_complex, 6

building_component, 6

building_part, 6

building_segment, 6

clustered_dispersion, 6

comparable, 2

composition_materials, 6

context, 1

CONTEXT, 6

context_sensibility, 5

continental, 6

database, 8

decision making institutions, 8

design research, 1

Design research, 7

design_research, 9

designing study, 7

designing_study, 7; 9

desirable, 3

district, 6

diversity of solutions, 2

documentation, 8

element, 6

ensemble, 6

flexibility, 2

form, 5

frame, 2; 5; 6

global, 6

grain, 2; 5; 6

hamlet, 6

historical images, 8

identifying design, 7

Identifying design, 7

impacts, 7

legend, 5

local, 6

material, 6

modality, 3

multifunctionality, 2

naming and defining, 8

national, 6

neighbourhood, 6

nominal_radius, 6

paradigmas, 2

perspective, 1; 7

PERSPECTIVE, 6

plan_horizon, 7

possible, 1; 3

predicted, 3

probable, 1; 3

problems, 4

programme_of_demands, 7

regional, 6

relations, 5

research by design, 1

Research by design, 7

robustness, 2

scale, 2

scale_level, 5

scale_paradox, 5

scope, 5

selection, 8

sentence functions, 8

site, 5

studying_design, 9

sub-continental, 6

subelement, 6

sub-national, 6

sub-regional, 6

superelement, 6

syntactically composed keywords, 8

tolerances, 5

trade_materials, 6

types, 7

typological_research, 9

typology, 7

unique, 2

variables, 5

variants, 7

village, 6


 

 



[a] [Bacon, 1620; 1854 #931], Praefatio, cited by (Kant 1787)  in his preface, states that science has not to be concerned as opinion, but as work (‘…non Opinionem, sed Opus…’). Elsewhere Bacon states that nature has to be forced to answer the question of the scientist. The scientist firstly has to design the experiment in order to produce improbable events in  some future. Kant states in his preface that scientists before Bacon understood that human reason only recognizes what it produces itself by design (‘…das die Vernunft nur das einsieht, was sie selbst nach ihrem Entwurf hervorbringt.’).

[b] (Kuhn 1962) stresses the role of instrument design in the change of scientific paradigmas. Van der Meer got a Nobel prize for his improvement of the cyclotron.  It only made new empirical discoveries possible.

[c] ‘A future’ is a set of imaginated events. Events have a lower level of abstraction than the concept of future. The imaginated events and their effects can differ, so we speak about futures in plural. The way we speak about different types of future or past is again on a higher level of abstraction modality.

[d] These conditionals are presupposed in the  conditionals from formal logic as defined by the truth table. Therefore they must be of another nature. I did not study modal logic to clarify them further.

[e] Pattern recognition techniques still do not recognize design relevant characteristics.

[f] IAAI(registration) for instance is a full sentence function that means ‘IAAI as a function of registration’ (Jong 2000; Jong and Graaf 2000; Jong and Rosemann 2000; Jong and Voordt 2000).

[g] Image Archive of Architectural Interventions by the Faculty of Architecture, University of technology Delft, The Netherlands. See the website ( http://iaai.bk.tudelft.nl/ ).